Program Ratings And Warning Labels example essay topic

3,165 words
This study used a 2 x 2 x 2 design to examine the effects of warning labels, bleeping, and gender on viewers' perceptions and enjoyment of a do cu-drama. We also examined the individual difference variable of verbal aggressiveness to test for possible interactions. Overall, the warning labels increased enjoyment of the program containing profanity among college students. Bleeping had no effect on either program liking or perceptions of realism; however, bleeping decreased perceptions of the program's offensiveness, and increased viewers' perceptions of profanity frequently estimates.

Lastly, verbally aggressive participants perceived the program as more realistic, and the language as less offensive The Telecommunications Act of 1996 mandated that broadcasters in the United States adopt program age and content ratings in order to help viewers make program viewing decisions. Since then, not only have program ratings and warning labels become a more familiar sight on television programs, but the very content that viewers are being warned about (e. g., profanity) appears to be occurring more frequently (Bauder, 2002). Research has examined the effect of age and content ratings and warning labels on children's program liking and perceptions of content, and found that in some circumstances, warnings and ratings have effects opposite those intended by the legislators (see, for example, Cantor & Harrison, 1996). Considerably less research has examined the effect on adults' perceptions of content (however, see Bushman, 1997).

Furthermore, a majority of the research examines the effects of ratings and warnings on violent content (e. g., Cantor & Harrison, 1996; Cantor, Harrison, & Nathanson, 1998; Herman & Leyens, 1977) or educational content (Krcmar & Albada, 2000). Little, if any research has examined the effect of ratings and warnings on attitudes toward, and perceptions of, other potentially objectionable material such as cursing. It may be interesting to ask, therefore, if assigning a warning label affects how adult viewers interpret and recall cursing when it appears in television programs. In addition to the use of program warnings, there are increasing numbers of cable channels available to viewers which may have served to loosen some norms regarding appropriate standards for programming. For example, in a recent airing of A Season on the Brink, over 100 instances of curse words (including those restricted by most network television; e. g., goddamn) were recorded.

Another airing of the same program self-censored (or 'bleeped') the profanity. Therefore, although program ratings have allowed viewers greater opportunity to filter their viewing, lowered norms of self-censorship have perhaps broadened the material available to them. These less restrictive norms, on one hand, and more frequent labeling of material, on the other, lead us to ask how labels and self-censoring affect the enjoyment and perceptions of program content by viewers. Therefore, the current study uses an experimental design to examine the effects of program labels and self-censoring on the perceptions and enjoyment of viewers. First, we will discuss the possible effects of warnings and self-censoring (bleeping) on viewers' enjoyment and perceptions of the content. Next, we will consider possible individual differences in viewers' responses to the manipulations.

The Effects of Program Ratings and Warning Labels Although few studies on program warnings had been conducted prior to the 1990's (see, however, Austin, 1980; Herman & Leyens, 1977; Wurtzel & Surlin, 1978), the current use of program warnings and ratings by the networks, combined with the increase in parental concern about problematic media content (Cantor, Stutman & Duran, 1996), has created an environment in which research into this issue has become more prevalent. A majority of these studies is grounded in reactance theory (Brehm, 1981). Reactance theory states that people are motivation ally aroused when they believe that their freedom or decision-making power is in some way threatened. To deal with this threat, people are motivated to regain their freedom, derogate the restrictive agent, or enhance the perceived value of the restricted item.

Based on this premise, it seems likely that restrictive program ratings (e. g., TV-14 for an 11-year old) and program warnings, which may be perceived by a viewer as limiting their decision-making power, to some extent, could generate reactance in the viewer. In other words, restrictive labels could enhance viewer enjoyment. Consistent with reactance theory, several studies have found that restrictive labels and warning labels do enhance viewers' enjoyment or selection of those programs (Bushman, 1997), especially for boys (Cantor & Harrison, 1996) and older children (Cantor & Harrison, 1996). In general, the research on viewers' responses to labels seems to suggest that children experience reactance to restrictive labels, especially in cases where the rating restricts their viewing (e. g., PG-13 for a child of 11). Reactance occurs less for children when simple labels are used (e. g., 'V'), most likely because no restriction is implied.

Therefore, reactance seems to occur with older children; however, some studies have also found support for reactance among adults (for counter evidence, see Austin, 1980; Christenson, 1992; Wurtzel & Surlin, 1978). For example, Herman and Leyens (1977) examined adult viewing patterns based on 4 years of Belgian television movies. Their main finding was that films carrying violence or sex advisories had larger audiences than those that did not. Their results, then, may seem to support reactance theory. However, because the study was archival, not experimental, movies that received advisories were systematically different from movies that did not have advisories.

This study may have demonstrated only that violent or sexual content with or without advisories attracts viewers. More valid evidence comes from a series of studies by Bushman and colleagues (Bushman & Stack, 1996; Bushman, 1997). Arguing that warning labels may also cause reactance, especially in adult viewers, Bushman and Stack (1996) used made-for-television movies and systematically varied the warning labels. When asked how much they liked the movies, audiences showed a preference for movies with warnings. This result was especially true among those participants who were initially high in reactance. In a second study, Bushman (1997) compared the responses of participants who had read identical movie descriptions with either a content rating, a warning label, or no label.

Overall, adult participants showed greater preference for the movie descriptions with a warning. Therefore, it appears that even among adult audiences, warning labels can affect enjoyment and selective exposure. Therefore, it appears that while age ratings can affect children, warning labels may encourage reactance in adults. In accord with reactance theory, the age-based (e. g., TV-14) rating information can induce higher levels of enjoyment from the programming content among the children who are younger than the specified age group rating because it implies prohibition.

With adult viewers, on the other hand, even the most stringent labels (e. g., TV-MA) do not restrict them. Therefore, for adults, it is the warning, not the rating, that implies restriction. By suggesting that the material may be too offensive for some viewers, warnings may imply that decisions should be made with caution, thereby limiting their freedom. Therefore, we predict: H 1: There will be a main effect for warning labels and self-censoring, such that those in the warning statement condition and the self-censored condition will like the program more than those in the no warning condition or uncensored condition. In addition to examining the effect of the manipulations on program liking, we also investigated the role of warnings and self-censorship on perceived appropriateness of the program content.

In the past, profanity was relatively uncommon on television. For example, there was less than one use of rough -- not even necessarily profane -- language per prime-time hour on all the broadcast networks during the 1989-1990 TV season. By the 1999-2000 season, there were nearly five per hour, according to a study by the Parents Television Council (2002). Although audiences claim that this kind of realism is acceptable in some instances, such as adult programming in appropriate time slots and in documentaries (ASA, BBC, BSC, & ITC, 2000), it is unclear how audience members actually respond. In a particularly vivid example of this debate, the weekend of March 9-10, 2002 saw the airing of two highly controversial programs. Several profanities were included in the footage on the CBS documentary about the World Trade Center attack, and ESPN's first-ever original movie, A Season on the Brink, contained numerous swear words in its docudrama about basketball coach Bobby Knight.

Although the decisions to include the profanities were debated within the two networks, ultimately both decided to include them. Mark Shapiro, an ESPN senior vice president, stated that the decision to include the profanities was made in order to increase the realism of the program. He stated, 'You couldn't produce a movie on Bobby Knight and use phrases like aw, shucks and golly gee. It just wouldn't be believable' (Bauder, 2002). Interestingly, ESPN aired an identical version with the curse words bleeped, indicating, at the very least, that the network recognized that some viewers might prefer the self-censored version. Despite these controversies, little, if any research has examined the effect of cursing on viewers' perceptions of offensiveness in language and realism.

Although the British Broadcasting Corporation (ASA et al., 2000) has tracked audiences' views of various expletives since 1998, no experiments have compared audience perceptions of programs with the expletives included compared to those with the expletives self-censored. In addition, the warnings themselves might affect how the expletives are viewed. Therefore, we ask: RI: Is there a main effect for the self-censoring of cursing, such that the uncensored version is perceived as more offensive by participants than the bleeped version? RQ 2: Is there a main effect for the self-censoring of cursing such that the uncensored version is perceived as more realistic by participants than the bleeped version? In addition to examining the effects of warnings and bleeping on enjoyment and perceptions, we were also interested in how people processed material that, first, contains profanity and, second, contains a warning about that profanity. In the following section, we take an information-processing approach in order to explore how audiences respond to these variations in content.

Information Processing of Television Content The information processing approach assumes that the mind attends to, uses, makes sense of, and organizes information systematically. The approach is 'mechanistic but poses [problems] at the functional level' (Pas hler, 1999, p. 6). In other words, researchers examine, in detail, the behaviors of individuals, varying the conditions systematically, in order to speculate about how information is processed. One aspect of information processing that has been studied extensively is that of attention. For example, it is now well accepted that various features of auditory stimuli (e. g., change in pitch, a novel tone) make attention more likely. In fact, these auditory variations are actually attended to automatically (Lang, Geiger, Strickwerda & Sumner, 1993; Underwood & Moray, 1971).

Even when directed to ignore information heard over one channel, listeners tend to process the 'ignored' information when it changes pitch (Treisman, 1964). Therefore, it seems likely that any variations in pitch or tone might actually draw attention, even involuntarily, from the listener. How is this related to media presentations of profanity? It is common practice to audibly self-censor or 'bleep' profanity.

In fact, A Season on the Brink contained more than 100 instances of 'bleeping' in its self-censored version. It may be, then, that bleeping curse words actually serves to draw attention to the cursing. Although the viewer may not hear the curse per se, they may attend even more to the language being used. Furthermore, in an experimental setting, prior warning also serves to increase the amount of attention paid to a stimulus (Lang et al., 1993). Therefore, it seems likely that a prior warning, such as a viewer advisory, may serve to increase the amount of attention paid to the profanity used. We predict: H 2: There will be a main effect for bleeping, such that those in the bleeped condition will perceive more frequent use of profanity than those in the non censored condition.

H 3: There will be a main effect for warnings such that those in the warning condition will perceive more frequent use of profanity than those in the no warning condition. Despite the fact that some processing is automatic (Cohen, Dunbar & McClelland, 1990), as argued earlier, it is also possible that some individual differences exist that are based on environmental or attitudinal differences. For example, a loud noise that may startle someone who typically spends a great deal of time in a quiet environment (a librarian, for example) might go ignored by someone accustomed to loud noise (a factory worker, for example). Similarly, profanity may typically be processed automatically and ignored by someone who hears it frequently, but not by someone who is unaccustomed to it. In the case of the latter example, we might expect that for those who use profanity themselves and are therefore largely inured to it, self-censoring curse words could draw even greater attention to the cursing itself. One individual difference variable that measures use of profanity is verbal aggressiveness (Infante & Rancer, 1996).

Verbal aggressiveness may play a role in determining just who is affected by hearing profanity and who is not. In other words, processing may be automatic in some cases, but under certain conditions, or for certain people (i. e., those high in verbal aggression), the process may be brought to conscious awareness. The personality trait of verbal aggressiveness has been defined as a stable trait that reflects a person's tendency to attack 'a person's self-concept to deliver psychological pain' (Infante & Rancer, 1996, pp. 315-316). Furthermore, several of the items that measure verbal aggressiveness tap into an individuals' tendency to use profanity and insulting and strong language.

Verbal aggressiveness is considered a highly destructive form of communication behavior (Infante, 1995) and is designed to threaten and to intentionally inflict harm. Research focusing on verbal aggressiveness has shown that it damages the receiver's self-esteem and serves as a catalyst to inter spousal violence (e. g., Infante, Chandler, & Rudd 1989). Thus, verbal aggressiveness as a personality characteristic predicts a variety of behaviors from alcohol consumption (see Graham, Schmidt, & Gillis, 1996; Moss & Kirisci, 1995; Rohsenow & Bachorowski, 1984) to physical aggressiveness and substance use (Bukstein, 1996). It seems likely, therefore, that those who are verbally aggressive and who themselves use profane language, may be less likely to attend to and be offended by it. In fact, those who are high in verbal aggressiveness, as compared to their less verbally aggressive counterparts, may perceive fewer instances of profanity in the uncensored condition and find it both more appropriate and more realistic. Therefore: H 4: There will be an ordinal interaction between verbal aggressiveness and the self-censorship manipulation on viewer perceptions of how frequently profanity is used, such that those low in verbal aggression in the non censored condition will perceive more profanity, more offensiveness, and less realism than those high in verbal aggression.

MethodsParticipantsParticipants were 208 undergraduate students (92 men, 116 women) at a large northeastern university. They received extra course credit in exchange for their voluntary participation. They ranged in age from 17 to 25 (M = 19.5, SD = 1.38). Procedures After informed consent was obtained, participants watched the ESPN program, A Season on the Brink, about basketball coach Bobby Knight. It was edited to be 30 minutes long (the original movie was 90 minutes) and contained 76 incidents of curse words being spoken (or bleeped). Participants were randomly assigned to watch one of four versions of the movie.

The four versions were created by crossing two factors: Warning Label ('Due to strong adult language, viewer discretion is advised' versus no warning), and Censorship (swear words spoken versus bleeped out). While watching the movie, participants were asked to avoid interacting with others in order to eliminate possible effects of co-viewing; no interaction during viewing or questionnaire completion was observed. Edited clips were used in the experiment instead of showing the entire movie in order to keep the experiment to a manageable length of time and to prevent fatigue. The clips were edited so that they would be identical in terms of scenes chosen. Scenes were chosen that both focused on the storyline about the relationship between Bobby Knight and the players and that contained many swear words. In other words, the edited 30-minute clip was a condensed version of the whole movie and retained the natural flow of the storyline.

The manipulated material was based on a story of a well-known basketball coach. Therefore, there was a concern for the possible confounding effect of participants' familiarity and knowledge of Bobby Knight on perceived realism and perceived likeability. In other words, if a participant is already familiar with the real life story of how Knight conducted his coaching on and off the court and how and why he was fired from Indiana University, then familiarity and knowledge in the subject matter may affect how realistic the individual perceives the movie to be, regardless of the use of language, which is the focus of this study. Meanwhile, if a participant is already familiar with the real life story of Knight, which indicates his / her interests in college basketball, this could influence how likable's / he will find the movie. In order to control for this possible influence, several items measured the participants' familiarity with Bobby Knight's story and college basketball. These items were used as controlling variables in analyses, as explained below.

Moreover, the manipulated material was taken from a televised movie. Thus 29% of participants (60 out of total 208) had already seen the movie before they were exposed to the manipulation in the experiment. Two strategies were undertaken to deal with this issue. First, familiarity (i. e., having seen the movie previously) was used as a co variate in all analyses, as discussed. Second, additional analyses were done to consider this limitation.

T-tests were conducted to investigate the difference between those with and without previous exposure, in terms of the variables of interest (i. e., perceived likeability, realism, language offended ness, perceived frequency of profanity, and verbal aggressiveness). Significant differences were found in language offended ness and verbal aggressiveness. Those with previous exposure rated the language less offensive (M = 2.56, SD = . 94) than those without previous exposure (M = 2.97, SD = 1.01), t (202) = 2.70, p